do birds have a brain

References and Notes1E. L. MacLean et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

eLetters is a forum for ongoing peer review. eLetters are screened but not edited, proofread, or indexed. eLetters should provide substantive and scholarly commentary on the article. Figures with embedded content cannot be submitted, and using figures in eLetters is generally discouraged. If a figure is necessary, kindly provide a link to it within the eLetter’s body. Please read our Terms of Service before submitting an eLetter.

Note: The article usage will be updated every day as soon as it becomes available, with a three- to four-day delay. This delay means that usage data will not be visible right away after publication.

Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.

Choose the format in which you wish to export this publication’s citation. Direct import.

  • Consciousness: A Technique for Behavioral Decisions, by Bjørn Grinde, Encyclopedia, 3, 1, (60–76), https://doi org/10. 3390/encyclopedia3010005.
  • The Veterinarian’s Guide to Animal Welfare, (339–356), Bonnie Beaver, Gail Golab, Wildlife animal welfare issues, https://doi org/10. 1016/B978-0-323-91018-7. 00009-9.
  • Irene M. Pepperberg, Consequences of wild cockatoos using tools and manufacturing them, Acquiring knowledge org/10. 3758/s13420-022-00524-6.
  • Executive Functions in Birds, Katarzyna Bobrowicz & Samuel Greiff, Birds, 3, 2, (184-220), https://doi org/10. 3390/birds3020013.
  • Advances in animal robots: A historical review and perspective, Heliyon, 8, 11, (e11499), Zhengyue Zhou, Hao Mei, Rongxun Li, Chenyuan Wang, Ke Fang, Wenbo Wang, Yezhong Tang, Zhendong Dai org/10. 1016/j. heliyon. 2022. e11499.
  • Irene M. Animal Cognition, 25, 6, (1679-1682), Pepperberg, Remarks on “Intra- and interspecific variation in self-control capacities of parrots in a delay of gratification task”, org/10. 1007/s10071-022-01644-2.
  • Lars P. Klimaschewski, Lars P. In Parkinsons and Alzheimers Today, Klimaschewski, Introduction to Brain Development: Why do We Need so Many Nerve Cells?, (1-26), https://doi org/10. 1007/978-3-662-66369-1_1.
  • Lars P. Klimaschewski, Lars P. Introduction to Human Development: Why Do We Need So Many Nervenzellen?, Parkinson und Alzheimer heute, (1-29), https://doi org/10. 1007/978-3-662-63392-2_1.
  • Thomas W. Holstein, Intelligence – Theories and Applications, (105-119), Intelligence: Evolutionary Biological Foundations and Perspectives, org/10. 1007/978-3-031-04198-3_5.
  • In Intelligence – Theories and Applications, Andreas Draguhn, On the Neurobiology of Intelligence, (25–44), https://doi org/10. 1007/978-3-031-04198-3_2.
  • See more

Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.

Access to Science is available to AAAS Members through their login, and users who have purchased individual subscriptions can access other journals in the Science family.

This article can be downloaded and printed for your own academic, research, and teaching purposes.

Buy a single issue of Science for just $15 USD.

Download this article as a PDF file

  • By
    • Morgan E. Wirthlin
    • Tobias A. Schmid
    • et al.
  • By
    • Kashish Singh
    • Clinton K. Lau
    • et al.
  • By
    • Abebe Fromsa
    • Katriina Willgert
    • et al.

Neuroscientist Suzana Herculano-Houzel of Vanderbilt University, who did not participate in either of the two recent papers but wrote a commentary accompanying them, argues that rather than the DVR being the neocortex, the entire pallium in mammals and birds has similar developmental origins and connectivity, and as such, [the pallia of both classes] should be considered equivalent structures. Stacho demonstrates how it can be deceptive to accept what the unaided eye perceives. ”.

Neuroscientist Harvey Karten postulated in the 1960s that the DVR was connected to the neocortex in some way. Yet it didn’t stick. Others later asserted that the DVR actually correlated with other areas of the mammalian brain, such as the amygdala, which is responsible for processing emotions among other things. According to Stacho, “one of the biggest disputes in the field of comparative neurobiology has probably been the theory about a DVR [correlation].” But his new work lends credibility to Karten’s original hypothesis.

The results, according to Stacho and his associates, also provide insight into the evolution of ancient animal brains. Rumbling around 320 million years ago, a reptile roamed the earth, serving as the last common ancestor of birds and mammals. Additionally, the team thinks that its brain was most likely a forerunner to the brains of the two lineages that diverged during evolution. According to Stacho, “nobody knows how exactly the last common ancestor’s brain looked like.” “Most likely, it wasn’t like the neocortex or the DVR. It was most likely something in between that led to the development of the wulst and DVR in birds and a six-layered neocortex in mammals. ”.

According to two studies that were just published in Science, birds’ brains are actually far more like our sophisticated primate brains than previously believed. For a long time, it was believed that the absence of a neocortex in the avian brain limited its functionality. Mammals have a massive, evolutionarily modern outer layer of the brain called the neocortex, which comprises most of what is referred to as the pallium in vertebrates overall and enables sophisticated cognition and creativity. Despite having a different shape, the new research demonstrates that birds do, in fact, have a brain structure similar to the neocortex. It turns out that the brain region is arranged cellularly similarly to the mammal cortex, which explains why many birds display sophisticated behaviors and skills that have long perplexed scientists. The latest research even implies that some birds may exhibit consciousness.

Presenting the gray square in six different intensities, including at the edge of the birds’ perception, was essential to the experiment. Lead author and neurobiologist Andreas Nieder and his associates were able to verify that the crows were in fact drawing from a subjective experience rather than merely performing conditioned responses to stimuli in this way.

AbstractThe term “birdbrain” used to be derogatory. But humans, with their limited brain size, should have known better than to use the meager proportions of the bird brain as an insult. Part of the cause for derision is that the mantle, or pallium, of the bird brain lacks the obvious layering that earned the mammalian pallium its “cerebral cortex” label. However, birds, and particularly corvids (such as ravens), are as cognitively capable as monkeys (

Get complete access to this article and view all of the available purchase options.

FAQ

How intelligent are birds?

Birds are actually very smart compared to mammals, though some species are definitely smarter than others. Ornithologist John K. Terres wrote that birds in the crow family have probably achieved “the highest degree of intelligence” found in any birds.

Do birds have a nervous system?

Birds have a highly developed nervous system and relatively large brains compared to other animals of similar body size. Their brains are organized into similar regions as mammals, including the cerebral cortex, cerebellum, and brainstem.

How are birds like humans?

An example of convergent traits shared by birds and humans is that they are both “warm-blooded” (or endothermic), even though their most recent common ancestor was “cold-blooded” (or ectothermic). Birds and humans also both have an efficient, four-chambered heart rather than a two or three-chambered one.

Do chickens have brains?

Yet birds, and chickens in particular, have very complex brains that process information in a specific way. Research has also shown that hens display intricate cognitive, social and emotional behaviours that put them on an intellectual par with primates. Not quite so bird-brained after all!